Systematic methodology

Systematic ethnology

 Systematic anthropology

Systematic linguistics

Population geogenetics

Systematic poetics

 Systematic folkloristics

 

 

Reformatorium

Prehistoric tribes

 Prehistoric races

Prehistoric languages

Prehistoric archaeology

  Prehistoric religions

Prehistoric folklore

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

*     Language taxonomy

*     Ethnic taxonomy

*     Europic

*     Nordic

*     Indic

*     Littoralic

*     Caucasic

*     Elamitic

*     Negric

*     Melanic

*     Tungic

*     Pelasgic

*     Cimbric

*     Turanic 

*     Ugro-Scythic

*     Uralo-Sarmatic

*     Lappic

*     Sinic

 

 

*       Spain    France

*       Italy     Schweiz

*       Britain    Celts

*       Scandinavia

*       Germany

*       Balts   Slavs

*       Greece

*       Thrace     Dacia

*       Anatolia

 

 

The Upper Palaeolithic Ancestors of Turanic, Turkic and Cimbric Languages

 Clickable terms are red on the yellow background

 

 

Table 1. The Systematic Glottogenesis of Human Language Families

 

 

Table 2. Palaeo-Turanids (33,000 BC) with microblades, rock shelters, throwing knives and Y-hg R*-M173

 

 

 

Language

Family

Reclassification

Culture

Ancestors

Pictish

Celtic

Orcado-Picto-Scottish Scythic

Megalithic, Scotland

Clactonian

Scottish

Celtic

Orcado-Picto-Scottish Scythic

Megalithic, Scotland

 

Orcadian

Celtic

Orcado-Picto-Scottish Scythic

Megalithic cairns, Skye, the Orkneys

 

Aquitanian

Vasconic

Franco-Vasconic

Ogro-Pictonic-Vasconic

Megalithic, Aquitania

Tayacian

Solutrean

Gascognian

Vasconic

Ogro-Pictonic-Vasconic

Megalithic, southwest Spain

Solutrean

Basque

Vasconic

Ogro-Vasconic

Megalithic, Spanish Pyrenees

Aterian

Nuraghic

 

Sardino-Vasconic

Sardinian Megalithic, 1900 BC

 

Paleo-Sardinian

 

Sardino-Vasconic

Sardinian Megalithic, 1900 BC

 

North Picene

 

Italo-Vasconic

Italian Megalithic

 

Goidelic

Celtic

Albano-Gaelic

Deverel-Rimbury culture, 1800 BC

British incinerators,

Incinerators

Gallaecian

Celtic

Hispano-Gaelic

African Mauretanian Alpinids, 1800 BC

Incinerators

Celt-Iberians

Celtic

Hispano-Gaelic + Iberian

African Mauretanian Alpinids, 1800 BC

Incinerators

Venetic

Celtic

Italo-Gallic

Epi-Gravettian, 33,000 BC

Incinerators

Gaulish

Celtic

Franco-Celtic

Epi-Gravettian, 33,000 BC

Incinerators

Celtic Germanic

Celtic

Germano-Celtic

Epi-Gravettian, 33,000 BC

Incinerators

Iberian

Celtic

Hispano-Iberic

Magdalenian reindeer hunters

Microlithic

Tyrsenian

Etruscan

Italo-Turanic

Remedello fossa graves, 3400 BC

Microlithic

Raetic

Etruscan

Italo-Turanic

Remedello necropoleis

Microlithic

Camunian

Etruscan

Italo-Turanic

Remedello fossa graves, 3400 BC

Microlithic

Sicanian

Etruscan

Sicilo-Turanic

Remedello fossa graves, 3400 BC

Microlithic

Punic

 

Afro-Punic

Phoenician diaspora, 800 BC

Microlithic

Tartessian

Celtic

Phoenician-Punic Turanic

Phoenician diaspora, 800 BC

Phoenicians

Halstattian

Celtic

Austrian Sarmatic

Hallstatt princely chariot burials, 800 BC

Ferrolithic

Noric

Celtic

Austrian Sarmatic

Hallstatt princely chariot burials, 800 BC

Ferrolithic

Oscan

Celtic

Italo-Sarmatic

Hallstatt princely chariot burials, 800 BC

Ferrolithic

Volscian

Celtic

Italo-Sarmatic

Hallstatt princely chariot burials, 800 BC

Ferrolithic

Illyrian

Celtic

Graeco-Pelasgic

Pelasgic ochre burials

Levalloisian

Ligurian

 

Italo-Pelasgic

Pelasgic ochre burials, 4000 BC

Levalloisian

Minoan

Greek

Eteo-Cretan

Pelasgic Sea Peoples

Levalloisian

Minoan

Greek

Elamitic +  Pelasgic

Elamitoid Cretan bull-leapers

Acheulean

Elymian

Etruscan

Sicilo-Elamitic

Elamitoid bull-fighters

Acheulean

 Table 2. Renaming European and Non- European Language Families

 (from P. Bělíček: The Analytic Survey of European Anthropology, Prague 2018, Map 5, p. 29)

 

The Origins of Palaeolithic Levalloisian Gracile Neanderthalers

   The bearers of the Mousterian culture are identified unambiguously with the classic Neanderthals called Homo sapiens neanderthalensis. Since their first skulls were excavated in Gibraltar and at Neanderthal near Düsseldorf in 1856, palaeoanthropologists have conducted disputes about whether they have survived in modern races or died out as an extinct offshoot. An unbiased consideration was harmed by comparison to more gracile varieties of Homo sapiens sapiens whose gentler physiognomy doomed the rugged Neanderthals to extinction. Their low foreheads and prominent eyebrow ridges fostered erroneous prejudices about their low mental capacity and made scholars judge them as an extinct regressive side-branch of man. They tended to emphasise their regressive features, prominent eyebrow arcs, low foreheads and receding chins but omitted their progressive traits, larger cranial capacity, stronger arms and great achievements in technology.

    Traditional approaches insist on the dogma of unilinear sapientisation and concentrate on Homo sapiens sapiens from Palestine without noticing his hybrid nature and derived origin. Recent studies (Day and Stringer 1982) trace his first sapient predecessors back to a single source, centre and place, to his original seats in east Africa about 120,000 years ago. They count with his early appearance at Omo I in Ethiopia and Border Cave in Swaziland (from 120,000 to 100,000 BC). They assume that from east Africa he moved to Palestine where his finds were excavated in Mugharet es-Skhūl and Jebel Qafzeh (92,000 BC). This cave man showed a prominent chin, a rounded occiput and a reduced torus supraorbitalis. He represented Early Moderns followed soon by Late Moderns (Nelson, Jurmain 1988: 558) who invaded Europe between 50,000 and 30,000 BC. This theory is confusing because the Palestinian settlements correspond to the Natufian culture of Levalloiso-Mousterian stamp and their inhabitants should be a mixed population of Early and Classic Neanderthals. Their assumed travels to Europe refer to the colonisation of Aurignacian cultures of Epi-Levalloisian descent. It radiated from their Caspian homeland but in Palestine it got an infusion of Neanderthal blood from surrounding Mousterian populations. Their gracile countenance made anthropologists perceive these hybrid tribes undeservedly as ‘mythical sapientisators’ of the world.

    The primary goal of palaeoanthropology is not to deal with recent hybridisation but to explain the prehistoric evolution from primary pure races. Its focus should be on a contrastive analysis distinguishing human races corresponding to the bearers of Mousterian, Levalloisian and Micoquian cultures. The first preliminary step taken usually distinguishes the Levalloisians as the Progressive or Early Neanderthals from Mousterians as Classic or Late Neanderthals.1 The second necessary step presupposes distinguishing various generations of Mousterian colonists into four temporal horizons:

Neanderthal A     Levalloisians: Homo sapiens aniensis (Sergii 1935)

Neanderthal B     Mousterians: Homo s. neanderthalensis (King 1864)

Neanderthal I       Clactonians: Swanscombe man, Choukoutien man

                           Homo steinheimensis (Berckhemer 1935)

Neanderthal II      Tayacians: Fontéchevade man, Ehringsdorf man

Neanderthal III    Mousterians: La Chapelle aux Saints, Le Moustier  

Neanderthal IV    Solutreans: Solutré skeletons.

   The comparative analysis of Neanderthals must count with general tendency to brachycephalisation that is due to mixing with Lapponoid races remarkable for prominent brachycephaly. The Mongoloids are generally believed to exhibit higher brachycephaly than most Negroid races but their skull indices range from mesocephaly typical of Tungids to moderate brachycephaly common to the Armenoid Mongolids with aquiline noses. Accordingly, the Mousterian skull indices rank higher than those of most Magdalenian and Aurignacian finds: Dordogne man 65.7, Brünn 68.2, Cro-Magnon 72.4, Galley Hill 63.4 (G. Schwalbe – E. Fischer2; V. P. Alekseyev – I. I. Goxman3). We assume that the original average of Mousterian skulls did not exceed the skull index 77.8 measured in Neanderthals from Teshik-Tash while Progressive Neanderthals of Levalloisian origin may be calibrated at less than 72 but owing to the subsequent brachycephalisation they rose to higher values observed among modern Mongolids and Tungids. A remarkable feature was their high, angled and prominent nose (M. H. Wolpoff4; H. Nelson – R. Jurmain5), reminiscent of modern aquiline varieties.

   In order to avoid confusion, we should give up labelling Neanderthals as various genera and species (Sinanthropus, Homo neanderthalensis) of extinct primates and treat them as racial varieties of man apt of mutual interbreeding. Inconvenient terms of palaeoanthropology should be dropped and replaced by those of archaeology (Mousterians, Solutreans) so as to unify their taxonomy.

   The Neanderthal skulls differ from Palaeo-Negroid finds clearly in low foreheads and long faces. The Rhodesian man from Broken Hill and Saldanha had a high face, strong eyebrow arcs and receding chins and mandibles. The Neanderthal man from Broken Hill was originally dated to 100,000 BP but this dating must be shifted to a later horizon. The Saldanha man comes from finds in the Makapansgat cave in Transvaal. An upper jaw of a 9-year-old Neanderthal baby was excavated at Tanger in Morocco. A part of a lower mandible was found at Dire-Dawa in Ethiopia. The modern Hottentots and Masais display a clearly Mongoloid type of physiognomy with high cheekbones, long face and even some traces of epicanthus. They fight their foes with leaf-shaped lances though they abandoned the technique of retouching and make them from metal now.

    Rhodesian man may be closely related to Steinheim man (from 250,000 to 200,000 BC), who probably imported Mousterian-type Tayacian artifacts to Europe and deserves to be greeted as a forerunner of Mousterian Neanderthals.1 The Steinheim skull was mutilated in the same way as that of Peking mans, which may be interpreted as an indirect token of their cannibalist practices. On the other hand, Swanscombe man as a probable protagonist of the Clactonian culture may be linked to the Levalloisian tradition propagated by a more gracile Homo sapiens. Their finds are, however, associated with much Acheulean industry due to mixing. The Mousterian tradition continued later into the Solutrean (from 22,000 to 18,000 BC) and Clovis and Folsom leaf-shape cultures (12,000 BC) in America. The only way that allows anthropology to avoid confusion consists in replacing misleading labels by genetic lineages such as Mousterian I-V, Levalloisian I-VI.

 

 

The Pelasgic and Turanic Retroflexive Consonants

 

   The heritage of European Pelasgians is hidden in clusters of neighbouring languages and may be reconstructed only by means of indirect parallels. Their racial and lexical component was significant in Bulgarian, Italian, French and Irish dialects and constituted the core of the Mediterranean race. The families of Mediterranean lake-dwellers, Pelasgic seafarers and mythical Hyperboreans in the Ukraine occupied their primary homes but owing to later invasions they lost their original homogeneous character. Their specific linguistic character and cultural customs have been preserved only in their main secondary domains in the east. Their largest compact refuge was represented by the Tungus fishermen and the Uto-Aztecan tribes in America.   

   The families of  Pelasgoid, Tungusoid, Turanic and Dravidians languages descend from the Levalloisian stock of the Early Neanderthalers or Denisovans, who were piscivorous fishers and lived in waterside post-dwellings on wooden piers. Their languages were remarkable for the opposition of aspirated fortis and lenis consonants. Another characteristic marker were retroflexive stop that often degenerated into consonantal diphthongs tl, dl, tr, dr. They were preserved best in the Dravidian languages of India but their remains are clearly visible in Lydian and Carian languages of Asia Minor and  in the Uto-Aztecan languages of America.

   The Greek Pelasgians recognised as their closest relatives Lelegs, Karians, Lydians and Palaites. Their original language is usually reconstructed from a few rare remains in Greek dialects such as the lexical suffixes -inthos and -issos, used in hyakinthos ‘hyacinth’ and kyparissos ‘cypress’ (Georgiev 1958; Katićić 1976). The original appearance of Pelasgian probably resembled Lydian, an Anatolian language with l-plurals and retroflexed laterals (Shevoroshkin 1967: 24). The Lydians separated as an independent nation under their ruler Gyges (692-654 BC). Their own autonym was Maiones written by the ancient Greeks as Мήονες  (Shevoroshkin 1967: 11).

    The Iranian family consists of pastoralists inhabiting dry arid grasslands and it hardly contains any ancient lake-dwellers except for the Munja and Pashai, whose language uses the plural ending -ēlā. The Pashai plural marker kuli is a less reliable trait of Palaeo-Bulgarian ancestry because it appears in many Indo-Iranian dialects (Yefimov, Edel’man 1978: 277). 

    An Iranian bridge must be presupposed as a station on the corridor to the Dravidian family. The Dravidian riverside fishermen and sea peoples consist of the Turcoid group (Malayam, Kannada, Old Tamil, Kurukh, Kui) with r-plurals and a Tungusoid group (Telugu, Tulu, New Tamil, Kolami) with l-plurals. The Tulu plural mēji-lu ‘tables’ adds a plural suffix -lu to sg. mēji. The same plural marker is attached to the Telugu plural gurrā-lu ‘horses. Modern Tamil uses plurals in -al` where Classic Tamil applied endings in –ār (Andronov 1962). Gadaba sg. ki – pl. kil ‘hands’ and Kolami buza-l ‘breasts’ illustrate the plural suffix -l. Kolami kand-l ‘eyes’ from sg. kan, Naiki kan-l and Purji kan-ul ‘eyes’ probably indicate a Palaeo-Bulgarian root kan ‘eye’ (Andronov 1978: 350-5). The same group of Dravidian languages tends to exhibit lambdacism and reproduce z` by l`. While r-Dravidian languages Kui, Kuvi, Braui, Konda and Gadaba display the rhotacism  z` > r`,  Modern Tamil, Tulu, Kolami carry out a lambdacism z` > l (Andronov 1978: 340).

    In r-Dravidian dialect of India most towns are given names by compounds with pur ‘town, stronghold’ (Nagpur, Kanpur, Hastinapur). But in Tirišira-ppalli its substitute is the Malayalam word palli ‘town‘. This word is derived from Pelasgic and later also common Greek -polis ‘town‘, Thracian poltys ‘castle’ and possibly also the Evenk bilēk ‘village’ (Cincius 1952: 130). Dravidian languages contain greater numbers of Turkic lexical cognates than the Tungus family. A clear Palaeo-Bulgarian root pel- may be seen in the Latin pelles ‘cat‘, Persian pelang ‘tiger’, Telugu pilli ‘cat‘ and puli ‘tiger‘ (Caldwell 1913: 599-600). Another is suggested by the Latin canis ‘dog’, Irish , Evenk ngen ‘dog’ and Aztec bān ‘coyote’  (Cincius 1952: 574).

      The opposition of rhotacism and lambdacism functions as a distinctive trait evident in the ethnonymic pairs Tur-, Dravid-, Tulu, Telugu. It also helps to distinguish two branches of Polynesian seafarers. One Turcoid group translated the Altaic god of heavens Torgut as Tagarro while the other Tungusoid branch called the same god Tagalo. The Tungusoid branch did not come from India but proceeded from the Tungus area through the Dungan and      Fu-tsiang languages in China to Formosan. Tavalon or Taparon in Taiwan (Yazyki i dialekty mira 1982: 53). Their travels continued by Indonesian   languages Tagal and Tagalog and resulted in the rise of Polynesian Tonga.

      Reconstructing residual dialects presupposes dissecting them from the dominant superstratum according to a few characteristic residual traits. Pelasgoid and Tungusoid dialects may be delimited roughly as a group of languages with l-plurals, lambdacism, alternations d/t/s/r > 1, four laterals with retroflexed pronunciation, ‘dzekanyede > dz and futuropraesentia with b-markers. Their characteristic sounds are retroflexed laterals dl, tl pronounced as lateral diphones (consonant affricates) dl, tl or consonant clusters dl, tl. Their structure tends to lay the stress on the penultimate and exhibit vertical vowel harmony. Linguistic comparison must, however, be preceded by reliable cultural parallels. Reliable typological criteria can be seen in pile-dwellings, cave burials, menhir tombstones and mollusc necklaces. The anthropological evidence of Tungids is less conspicuous, it rests on epicanthus, leptorrhinia, gracile countenance and higher rates of blood groups B.

 

(from P. Bělíček: Prehistoric Dialects, Prague 2004, pp. 559-401)

 

 

 

The Pulmonic Sound-Repertory of Perigordian, Turanic, Turcoid and Dravidiand Languages

   On the other hand, Altaic small-game pastoralists and nomadic fishers created a specific pulmonic phonology relying on front rounded vowels and the correlation of fortis and lenis plosives (Table 6). They spoke consonantal pulmonic languages1 produced by the air pressure coming from the lungs with different degrees of explosive and aspirative force. According to their tension and explosive charge, their consonants split into fortis and lenis phonemes. Their vowels distinguished tense and lax counterparts, too. They were semantically irrelevant as they harmonised the syllabic relief by vocalic synharmony. Its purpose was to balance series of consonantal clusters infilled with front or back rounded vowels. Their pronunciation was often mutated by retroflex colouring. Their stock was divided into Turanids with apical retroflex rhotacism and Tungids with laminal retroflex rhotacism.

Apical pulmonic pressure languages

Turcoid languages with apical retroflex sounds

Laminal pulmonic pressure languages

Tungusoid languages with laminal retroflex sounds

rounded vowels: o u ö ü

unrounded vowels: ä e i

front vowels: ä e    i ö ü

back vowels: a ı í o u

rounded vowels: o u ö ü

unrounded vowels: ä e i

front vowels: ä e    i ö ü

back vowels: a ı  í o u

tense vowels: a: i: u:

lax vowels: a i u

rhotic vowels: ar, ur, ir

tense vowels: a: i: u:

lax vowels: a i u

pharyngealised vowels: al, ul, il

fortis plosives: p t k

aspirated plosives: ph- th- kh-

fortis fricatives: f   s χ h

fortis approximants: j w

fortis plosives: p t k

aspirated plosives: ph- th- kh

fortis fricatives: f   s χ h

fortis approximants: j w

lenis  plosives: b̥ d̥ g̊

lenis plosive geminates: -bb- -dd- -gg-

lenis nasals: m̥ n̥ ŋ̊

lenis plosives: b̥ d̥ g̊ p͈ t͈ 

lenis plosive geminates: -bb- -dd- -gg-

lenis nasals: m̥ n̥ ŋ̊

apical phonemes: ṭ ḍ ṇ ṣ ẓ ḷ  ɾ̣ ɹ ̣ or ʈ ɖ ɳ ʂ ʐ ɭ ɻ ɽ

laminal phonemes: ṭ ḍ ṇ ṣ ẓ ḷ  ɾ̣ ɹ̣ or ʈ ɖ ɳ  ʂ ʐ  ɭ ɻ ɽ

r-affricates: pr tr dr kr

l-affricates pl, tl, dl kl

cacuminal/apical flap  ɽ

laminal flap ɭ̆ 

stress, accent on the ultimate syllable

rhyming  or parallelistic consonance prosody

stress, accent on the penultimate syllable

rhyming  or parallelistic consonance prosody

Table 6.  The pulmonic phonology of Turcoids and Tungids with flake-tool industry

   Elementary grammatical systems fall into three types of nominal and verbal morphology. The gender-oriented morphology is attributable to the language family of tall dolichocephals with hand-axe industry and vegetal subsistence. In its original appearance documented in African, Melanesian and Australian Negrids it partitioned nouns into classes of animate, inanimate, vegetal and arboreal classes. These classed were distinguished by prefixes put in front of nouns. In the Horn of Africa their family ran upon Asiatic races with agglutinating language structures and transitioned to suffixing morphology of inflecting type. The group of Asiatic plant-gatherers, hoe-cultivators and agriculturalists reduced the system of twelve nominal classifiers to the opposition of animate and inanimate nouns. Their category included humans, animals, animistic spirits as well as sacral deities.

This categorisation survived also in Anatolian tongues until their further expansion in the Balkans encountered Gravettian tribes of Alpinids with sex-based gender classifications. Their clash resulted in the rise of sex-based nominal gender enriched by masculine o-stems and feminine a-stems. The core of European Gothids accepted the dual opposition of masculine and feminine gender but their core remained reluctant to their addition and continued to adhere to nominal i-stems. Their subclasses coexisted with Caucasoid vegetal u/w-stems that can be explained as remains of Caucasoid b-plurals referring to agricultural crops and instruments of farming activities. The classification of Indo-European thematic and athematic stems may be regarded as a hold-over of ancient invasions and infiltrations surviving in residual form in the territory of Europe. The sex-based gender distinction of the suffixes -o and -a first appeared in African Chadic and Ethiopian Galla languages and their spread all over Europe was due to the Gravettian colonisation of short-sized brachycephals to the north. They were embedded into the system of IE accidence as new thematic stems distinguishing the masculine o-stems and feminine a-stems. The u-stems penetrated into the IE word stock with the propagation of Neolithic farming from the Fertile Crescent to the Danubian river basin.

  The so-called IE t-stems are reserved for animal species and prevail in terms for pastoralist herding and animal husbandry. They must have been imported from Uralic languages with t-plurals by means of Sarmatian raiders and Hallstattian colonists. A similar account may be given to r-stems that append the marker -r in Latin sg. genus as opposed to pl. genera. Their origin may be hypothesised as an import of Mesolithic Turcoid tribes with microlith flake-tools and r-plural. The occurrence of r-plurals and umlaut change in German Bach – Bächer, Buch – Bücher is incorrectly elucidated as a consequence of rhotacism -s > -r without seeing parallels to Turcoid pluralisation and vowel harmony.

Article-oriented nominalisation

Bascoids with articles and

category of determination

Case-oriented morphology

Altaic Turcoids with

agglutinating language structures

Case-oriented morphology

Siberian Tungids with

agglutinating language structures

suffixing agglutination

suffixing agglutination

suffixing agglutination

no gender categories

no gender categories

no gender categories

category of determination

indefinite and definite articles

no articles

no articles

Ergative constructions with abso-lutive, oblique and ergative case

locative subcategorisation of

cases into essives and allatives

nominative vs. accusative con-

structions with locative cases

plural and dual number: -k -t

Bascoid: distinctive k-plurals

Uraloid collective t-plurals

number: singular plural

Turcoid r-plurals

number: singular plural

Tungusoid l-plurals

 

possession: possessive prefixes

possession: possessive suffixes

possession: possessive suffixes

cases: prefixing case markers

ergative – absolutive

cases: suffixing case markers

nominative - accusative

cases: suffixing case markers

nominative – accusative

word order: OVS, SOV

adjective attributes: NA

nominal attributes: GN

numeral attribution: NumN

word order: SOV

adjective attributes: AN

nominal attributes: GN

numeral attribution NumN

word order: SOV

adjective attributes: AN (NA)

nominal attributes: GN

numeral attribution NumN

adjunctions: prepositions

conjuctions: prejunctions

adjunctions: postpositions

conjuctions: postjunctions

adjunctions: prepositions,

conjuctions: prejunctions

analytic semipredication with

gerunds, infinitives and participles

semipredication with gerunds, infinitives and participles

analytic semipredication with

gerunds, infinitives and participles

stress: accent on initial syllables

accent on ultimate syllables

accent on penultimate syllables

versification: alliterative

prosody: rhyming consonance

parallelistic consonance

Table 9.  The morphology of Asiatic races with flake-tool industry

   The centre point of Asiatic language families lies in the categories of case, determination, state and possession. Table 9 proposes a typological classification of Non-Indo-European language structures that encapsulated from without into their lexical substance. The left column sums Abkhaz, Scythoid, Ugroid language types into the Bascoid family of article-oriented dialects. Their family is usually counted as a member of the Altaic Sprachbund although it diverges as an independent subtype.

 

(from P. Bělíček: The Analytic Survey of European Anthropology, Prague 2018, p. 35-42)

 

 



1 J. Buettner-Janusch: Physical Anthropology: A Perspective. New York - London: Wiley, 1973, p. 253.

2 G. Schwalbe – E. Fischer: Anthropologie. Leipzig 1923, p. 286.

3 V. P. Alekseyev I. I. Goxman: Antropologiya aziatskoy chasti SSSR. Moskva 1984, p. 9.

4 M. H. Wolpoff: Palaeoanthropology. New York 1980, p. 280.

5 H. Nelson R. Jurmain: Introduction to Physical Anthropology. New York - Los Angeles, 1988, p. 540.

1 H. Nelson R. Jurmain: Introduction to Physical Anthropology. New York - Los Angeles, 1988, p. 532.

1 Ian Maddieson: Presence of uncommon consonants. In: Martin Haspelmath – M. S. Dryer – David Gil – Bernard Comrie (eds.) The World Atlas of Language Structures Online. Munich: Max Planck Digital Library, 2008; Ian Maddieson: Glottalic Consonants. In: Martin Haspelmath – M. S. Dryer  – David Gil  – Bernard Comrie (eds.) The World Atlas of Language Structures Online. Munich: Max Planck Digital Library, 2008.